LightNovesOnl.com

Human. Part 12

Human. - LightNovelsOnl.com

You're reading novel online at LightNovelsOnl.com. Please use the follow button to get notifications about your favorite novels and its latest chapters so you can come back anytime and won't miss anything.

25. s.e.m.e.ndeferi, K., Lu, A., Schenker, N., and Damasio, H. (2002). Humans and great apes share a large frontal cortex. Nature Neuroscience 5: 27276.

26. s.e.m.e.ndeferi, K., Damasio, H., Frank, R., and Van Hoesen, G.W. (1997). The evolution of the frontal lobes: A volumetric a.n.a.lysis based on three-dimensional reconstructions of magnetic resonance scans of human and ape brains. Journal of Human Evolution 32: 37588.

27. s.e.m.e.ndeferi, K., Armstrong, E., Schleicher, A., Zilles, K., and Van Hoesen, G.W. (2001). Prefrontal cortex in humans and apes: A comparative study of area 10. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 114: 22441.

28. Schoenemann, P.T., Sheehan, M.J., and Glotzer, L.D. (2005). Prefrontal white matter volume is disproportionately larger in humans than in other primates. Nature Neuroscience 8: 24252.

29. Damasio, A. (1994). Descartes' Error. New York: Putnam.



30. Johnson-Frey, S.H. (2003). What's so special about human tool use? Neuron 39: 2014.

31. Johnson-Frey, S.H. (2003). Cortical mechanisms of tool use. In Johnson-Frey, S.H. (ed.), Taking Action: Cognitive Neuroscience Perspectives on the Problem of Intentional Movements (pp. 185217). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

32. Johnson-Frey, S.H., Newman-Morland, R., and Grafton, S.T. (2005). A distributed left hemisphere network active during planning of everyday tool use skills. Cerebral Cortex 15: 68195.

33. Buxhoeveden, D.P., Switala, A.E., Roy, E., Litaker, M., and Casanova, M.F. (2001). Morphological differences between minicolumns in human and nonhuman primate cortex. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 115: 36171.

34. Casanova, M.F., Buxhoeveden, D., and Soha, G.S. (2000). Brain development and evolution. In Ernst, M., and Rumse, J.M. (eds.), Functional Neuroimaging in Child Psychiatry (pp. 11336). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

35. Goodhill, G.J., and Carreira-Perpinan, M.A. (2002). Cortical columns. In Encyclopedia of Cognitive Science. Basingstoke, UK: Macmillan.

36. Marcus, J.A. (2003). Radial Neuron Number and Mammalian Brain Evolution: Rea.s.sessing the Neocortical Uniformity Hypothesis. Boston: Doctoral dissertation, Department of Anthropology, Harvard University.

37. Mountcastle, V.B. (1957). Modality and topographic properties of single neurons of cat's somatic sensory cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology 20: 40834.

38. Buxhoeveden, D.P., and Casanova, M.F. (2002). The minicolumn hypothesis in neuroscience. Brain 125: 93551.

39. Jones, E.G. (2000). Microcolumns in the cerebral cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 97: 501921.

40. Mountcastle, V.B. (1997). The columnar organization of the neocortex. Brain 120: 70122.

41. Barone, P., and Kennedy, H. (2000). Non-uniformity of neocortex: Areal heterogeneity of NADPH-diaphorase reactive neurons in adult macaque monkeys. Cerebral Cortex 10: 16074.

42. Beaulieu, C. (1993). Numerical data on neocortical neurons in adult rat, with special reference to the GABA population. Brain Research 609: 28492.

43. Elston, G.N. (2003). Cortex, cognition and the cell: New insights into the pyramidal neuron and prefrontal function. Cerebral Cortex 13: 112438.

44. Preuss, T. (2000a). Preface: From basic uniformity to diversity in cortical organization. Brain Behavior and Evolution 55: 28386.

45. Preuss, T. (2000b). Taking the measure of diversity: Comparative alternatives to the model-animal paradigm in cortical neuroscience. Brain Behavior and Evolution 55: 28799.

46. Marin-Padilla, M. (1992). Ontogenesis of the pyramidal cell of the mammalian neocortex and developmental cytoarchitectonics: A unifying theory. Journal of Comparative Neurology 321: 22340.

47. Caviness, V.S.J., Takahas.h.i.+, T., and Nowakowski, R.S. (1995). Numbers, time and neocortical neurogenesis: A general developmental and evolutionary model. Trends in Neuroscience 18: 37983.

48. Fuster, J.M. (2003). Neurobiology of cortical networks. In Cortex and Mind (pp. 1753). New York: Oxford University Press.

49. Jones, E.G. (1981). Anatomy of cerebral cortex: Columnar input-output organization. In Schmitt, F.O., Worden, F.G., Adelman, G., and Dennis, S.G. (eds.), The Organization of the Cerebral Cortex (pp. 199235). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

50. Hutsler, J.J., and Galuske, R.A.W. (2003). Hemispheric asymmetries in cerebral cortical networks. Trends in Neuroscience 26: 42935.

51. Ramon y Cajal, S. (1990). The cerebral cortex. In New Ideas on the Structure of the Nervous System in Man and Vertebrates (pp. 3572). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

52. Elston, G.N., and Rosa, M.G.P. (2000). Pyramidal cells, patches and cortical columns: A comparative study of infragranular neurons in TEO, TE, and the superior temporal polysensory area of the macaque monkey. Journal of Neuroscience 20: RC117: 15.

53. Hutsler, J.J., Lee, D.-G., and Porter, K.K. (2005). Comparative a.n.a.lysis of cortical layering and supragranular layer enlargement in rodent, carnivore, and primate species. Brain Research 1052: 7181.

54. Caviness, V.S.J., Takahas.h.i.+, T., and Nowakowski, R.S. (1995). Numbers, time and neocortical neurogenesis: A general developmental and evolutionary model. Trends in Neuroscience 18: 37983.

55. Hutsler, J.J., Lee, D.-G., and Porter, K.K. (2005). Comparative a.n.a.lysis of cortical layering and supragranular layer enlargement in rodent, carnivore, and primate species. Brain Research 1052: 7181.

56. Darlington, R.B., Dunlop, S.A., and Finlay, B.L. (1999). Neural development in metatherian and eutherian mammals: Variation and constraint. Journal of Comparative Neurology 411: 35968.

57. Finlay, B.L., and Darlington, R.B. (1995). Linked regularities in the development and evolution of mammalian brains. Science 268: 157884.

58. Rakic, P. (1981). Developmental events leading to laminar and areal organization of the neocortex. In Schmitt, F.O., Worden, F.G., Adelman, G., and Dennis, S.G. (eds.), The Organization of the Cerebral Cortex (pp. 728). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

59. Rakic, P. (1988). Specification of cerebral cortical areas. Science 241: 17076.

60. Ringo, J.L., Doty, R.W., Demeter, S., and Simard, P.Y. (1994). Time is of the essence: A conjecture that hemispheric specialization arises from interhemispheric conduction delay. Cerebral Cortex 4: 33134.

61. Hamilton, C.R., and Vermeire, B.A. (1988). Complementary hemisphere specialization in monkeys. Science 242: 169194.

62. Cherniak, C. (1994). Component placement optimization in the brain. Journal of Neuroscience 14: 241827.

63. Allman, J.M. (1999). Evolving brains. Scientific American Library Series, No. 68. New York: Scientific American Library.

64. Hauser, M., and Carey, S. (1998). Building a cognitive creature from a set of primitives: Evolutionary and developmental insights. In c.u.mmins, D., and Allen, C. (eds.), The Evolution of the Mind (pp. 51106). New York: Oxford University Press.

65. Funnell, M.G., and Gazzaniga, M.S. (2000). Right hemisphere deficits in reasoning processes. Cognitive Neuroscience Society Abstracts Supplements 12: 110.

66. Rilling, J.K., and Insel, T.R. (1999). Differential expansion of neural projection systems in primate brain evolution. NeuroReport 10: 145359.

67. Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Gallese, V., and Foga.s.si, L. (1996). Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. Cognitive Brain Research 3: 13141.

68. Rizzolatti, G. (1998). Mirror neurons. In Gazzaniga, M.S., and Altman, J.S. (eds.), Brain and Mind: Evolutionary Perspectives (pp. 10210). HFSP workshop reports 5. Strasbourg: Human Frontier Science Program.

69. Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness. An Essay on Autism and Theory of Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

70. Watanabe, H., et al. (2004). DNA sequence and comparative a.n.a.lysis of chimpanzee chromosome 22. Nature 429: 38288.

71. Vargha-Khadem, F., et al. (1995). Praxic and nonverbal cognitive deficits in a large family with a genetically transmitted speech and language disorder. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 92: 93033.

72. Fisher, S.E., et al. (1998). Localization of a gene implicated in a severe speech and language disorder. Nature Genetics 18: 16870.

73. Lai, C.S., et al. (2001). A novel forkhead-domain gene is mutated in a severe speech and language disorder. Nature 413: 51923.

74. Shu, W., et al. (2001). Characterization of a new subfamily of winged-helix/forkhead (Fox) genes that are expressed in the lung and act as transcriptional repressors. Journal of Biological Chemistry 276: 2748897.

75. Enard, W., et al. (2002). Molecular evolution of FOXP2, a gene involved in speech and language. Nature 418: 86972.

76. Fisher, S.E. (2005). Dissection of molecular mechanisms underlying speech and language disorders. Applied Psycholinguistics 26: 11128.

77. Caceres, M., et al. (2003). Elevated gene expression levels distinguish human from non-human primate brains. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 100: 1303035.

78. Bystron, I., Rakic, P., Molnar, Z., and Blakemore, C. (2006). The first neurons of the human cerebral cortex. Nature Neuroscience 9: 88086.

Chapter 2: WOULD A CHIMP MAKE A GOOD DATE?.

1. Evans, E.P. (1906). The Criminal Prosecution and Capital Punishment of Animals. New York: E.P. Dutton.

2. International Human Genome Sequencing Consortium. (2001). Initial sequencing and a.n.a.lysis of the human genome. Nature 409: 860921; Errata 411: 720; 412: 565.

3. Venter, J.C., et al. (2001). The sequence of the human genome. Science 291: 130451. Erratum 292: 1838.

4. Watanabe, H., et al. (2004). DNA sequence and comparative a.n.a.lysis of chimpanzee chromosome 22. Nature 429: 382438.

5. Provine, R. (2004). Laughing, tickling, and the evolution of speech and self. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 13: 21518.

6. Benes, F.M. (1998). Brain development, VII: Human brain growth spans decades. American Journal of Psychiatry 155:1489.

7. Wikipedia.

8. Markl, H. (1985). Manipulation, modulation, information, cognition: Some of the riddles of communication. In Holldobler, B., and Lindauer, M. (eds.), Experimental Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology (pp. 16394). Sunderland, MA: Sinauer a.s.sociates.

9. Povinelli, D.J. (2004). Behind the ape's appearance: Escaping anthropocentrism in the study of other minds. Daedalus: The Journal of the American Academy of Arts and Sciences 133 (Winter).

10. Povinelli, D.J., and Bering, J.M. (2002). The mentality of apes revisited. Current Directions in Psychological Science 11: 11519.

11. Holmes, J. (1978). The Farmer's Dog. London: Popular Dogs.

12. Leslie, A.M. (1987). Pretense and representation: The origins of "theory of mind." Psychological Review 94: 41226.

13. Bloom. P., and German, T. (2000). Two reasons to abandon the false belief task as a test of theory of mind. Cognition 77: B25B31.

14. Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An Essay on Autism and Theory of Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

15. Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A.M., and Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a theory of mind? Cognition 21: 3746.

16. Heyes, C.M. (1998). Theory of mind in nonhuman primates. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21: 10134.

17. Povinelli, D.J., and Vonk, J. (2004). We don't need a microscope to explore the chimpanzee's mind. Mind & Language 19: 128.

18. Tomasello, M., Call, J., and Hare, B. (2003). Chimpanzees versus humans: It's not that simple. Trends in Cognitive Science 7: 23940.

19. White, A., and Byrne, R. (1988). Tactical deception in primates. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11: 23344.

20. Hare, B., Call, J., Agnetta, B., and Tomasello, M. (2000). Chimpanzees know what conspecifics do and do not see. Animal Behaviour 59: 77185.

21. Call, J., and Tomasello, M. (1998). Distinguis.h.i.+ng intentional from accidental actions in orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus), chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), and human children (h.o.m.o sapiens). Journal of Comparative Psychology 112: 192206.

22. Hare, B., and Tomasello, M. (2004). Chimpanzees are more skilful in compet.i.tive than in cooperative cognitive tasks. Animal Behaviour 68: 57181.

23. Melis, A., Hare, B., and Tomasello, M. (2006). Chimpanzees recruit the best collaborators. Science 313: 12971300.

24. Bloom, P., and German, T. (2000). Two reasons to abandon the false belief task as a test of theory of mind. Cognition 77: B25B31.

25. Call, J., and Tomasello, M. (1999). A nonverbal false belief task: The performance of children and great apes. Child Development 70: 38195.

26. Onis.h.i.+, K.H., and Baillargeon, R. (2005). Do 15-month-old infants understand false beliefs? Science 308: 25558.

27. Wellman, H.M., Cross, D., and Watson, J. (2001). Meta-a.n.a.lysis of theory of mind development: The truth about false-belief. Child Development 72: 65584.

28. Gopnik, A. (1993). How we know our minds: The illusion of first-person knowledge of intentionality. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16: 114.

29. Leslie, A.M., Friedman, O., and German, T.P. (2004). Core mechanisms in "theory of mind." Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8: 52833.

30. Leslie, A.M., German, T.P., and Polizzi, P. (2005). Belief-desire reasoning as a process of selection. Cognitive Psychology 50: 4585.

31. German, T.P., and Leslie, A.M. (2001). Children's inferences from "knowing" to "pretending" and "believing." British Journal of Developmental Psychology 19: 5983.

32. German, T.P., and Leslie, A.M. (2004). No (social) construction without (meta) representation: Modular mechanisms as the basis for the acquisition of an understanding of mind. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 27:1067.

33. Tomasello, M., Call, J., and Hare, B. (2003). Chimpanzees understand psychological states-the question is which ones and to what extent. Trends in Cognitive Science 7: 15456.

34. Povinelli, D.J., Bering, J.M., and Giambrone, S. (2000). Toward a science of other minds: Escaping the argument by a.n.a.logy. Cognitive Science 24: 50941.

35. Mulcahy, N., and Call, J. (2006). Apes save tools for future use. Science 312: 103840.

36. Anderson, S.R. (2004). A telling difference. Natural History 113 (November): 3843.

37. Chomsky, N. (1980). Human language and other semiotic systems. In Sebeokand, T.A., and Umiker-Sebeok, J. (eds.), Speaking of Apes: A Critical Anthology of Two-Way Communication with Man (pp. 42940). New York: Plenum Press.

38. Savage-Rumbaugh, S., and Lewin, R. (1994). Kanzi: The Ape at the Brink of the Human Mind. New York: Wiley.

39. Savage-Rumbaugh, S., Romski, M.A., Hopkins, W.D., and Sevcik, R.A. (1988). Symbol acquisition and use by Pan troglodytes, Pan paniscus, and h.o.m.o sapiens. In Heltne, P.G., and Marquandt, L.A. (eds.), Understanding Chimpanzees (pp. 26695). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

40. Seyfarth, R.M., Cheney, D.L., and Marler, P. (1980). Vervet monkey alarm calls: Semantic communication in a free-ranging primate. Animal Behaviour 28: 107094.

41. Premack, D. (1972). Concordant preferences as a precondition for affective but not for symbolic communication (or how to do experimental anthropology). Cognition 1: 25164.

42. Seyfarth, R.M., and Cheney, D.L. (2003). Meaning and emotion in animal vocalizations. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1000: 3255.

Click Like and comment to support us!

RECENTLY UPDATED NOVELS

About Human. Part 12 novel

You're reading Human. by Author(s): Michael S. Gazzaniga. This novel has been translated and updated at LightNovelsOnl.com and has already 1017 views. And it would be great if you choose to read and follow your favorite novel on our website. We promise you that we'll bring you the latest novels, a novel list updates everyday and free. LightNovelsOnl.com is a very smart website for reading novels online, friendly on mobile. If you have any questions, please do not hesitate to contact us at [email protected] or just simply leave your comment so we'll know how to make you happy.